01344 770892investigations@trl.co.uk
TwitterLinkedin
  • TRL Main Site
Top bar menu
Search
Expert Witness & Investigations ServicesExpert Witness & Investigations Services
Expert Witness & Investigations Services
Proven. Trusted. Effective.
  • Why Us
  • Our Services
    • Collision Investigation & Reconstruction
    • Rapid Response Scene & Vehicle Inspections
    • Initial Appraisal Service
    • CCTV / Dashcam Footage
    • GPS / Cycle Computer Analysis
    • Mechanical Vehicle Inspection
    • EDR / Black Box Downloads
    • Injury Causation, including Seat Belt and Helmet Usage
    • 2D Scene Plans
    • 3D Simulation
    • Driver & Vehicle Visibility Assessments
    • Vehicle Infotainment / Telematics System Downloads
    • Tachograph Analysis
    • Vehicle Fire Investigation
    • Vehicle Identification
    • Highways Investigations
    • Collision Investigation Training
  • The Experts
  • Testimonials
  • News
  • Contact Us
Menu back  
Expert Witness Home > Human Factors in Collision Investigation

Human Factors in Collision Investigation

June 16, 2022Collision Investigation & Reconstruction, Collision Investigation training, Expert Witness, Vehicle Identification, Vehicle safety

It is easy to forget sometimes with the advances in evolution and technology that we are all still animals! That is perhaps our weakest facet when driving or using the roads.  Another weakness is that, when we drive, most of the time we are moving beyond our ‘design speed’. Yes, drivers have literally overtaken evolution.

As innate as it is to be human and to go about our daily lives without considering how we function, it is when things go wrong, particularly in road collisions, that our design weaknesses come to the fore and we realise just how out of our comfort zone driving takes us.

Vehicle travel is on the whole comfortable, quiet and designed to limit fatigue; to the point that we are not generally aware just how fast we are travelling and how ill-adapted to it we are.

This is where the collision investigator should start to consider underlying reasons for our failings when it goes wrong.  It is important to remember that this is not to find excuses, but to identify possible reasons for the Courts and other fora to consider, in their judgements.

Everything, including our sight function, physiology (when it goes wrong), and cognitive processing power, becomes ever more compromised the faster above about 23 mph that we go – unless perhaps for Usain Bolt (28 mph for a few seconds). However, our rationalising mind struggles to notice this compromise when travelling at speed, having to assess the changing environment ahead and detect potential and real hazards needing action when sometimes it is too late. Here we will explore some of these human factors in a bit more detail.

Sight – we are ‘daytime’ or diurnal creatures.  Our eyes developed for this, giving us good trichromatic colour vision.  At night, our problems come from the great concentration of colour detection cells (cones) at and around the centre of our forward field of view (FFOV). That is ideal for daytime, but our low light and night-time, black and white detection cells (rods) are few in the central field of view and offer limited assistance in low light levels, except more toward our peripheral field of view.  We are grateful for vehicle lighting, but how effective really is our visual detection toward the edges and beyond of the lit area ahead of us on the road?

 

Contrast and conspicuity – two different but very much linked factors. Contrast is the visual grade between two tones of colour, whether monochrome, or in trichromatic colour.

To detect an object discrete from its background requires it to be sufficiently distinct in contrast gradient for the eye to detect it as being such (about 10% – Weber-Fechner Law). This applies in any lighting level. Contrast is an element to assist with detection, along with conspicuity, the latter being that quality of an object to make it detectable. This is also what makes the object ‘important enough’ for us to look for it and to detect it.

The golfer on the fairway sees their ball in the long grass, readily in view, because they are looking for it, whereas the non-golfer may not detect it, as the ball is unimportant and not of interest to them. This links with expectancy, or what we expect to see in a given situation – more on this later.

Furthermore, our brain is the ‘control unit’ in charge of the laziest machine known to humankind – us! Naturally, if it can find ways to shortcut a process, save energy and time, it will – unless we are aware of this and condition to override it.

Why do emerging right-turn drivers often fail to see vehicles, particularly motorcycles, approaching from their right, until it is too late, or worse, at impact?

One reason could be change blindness, aside from detection.  Essentially the brain acknowledges the data from the initial right observation, before turning toward the left. Having not acknowledged anything in their initial right observation that required immediate attention, it is understood the brain ‘remembers’ the initial observation and short-cuts the second right observation.  Research has found that up to a quarter of the scene in the observation can have changed, without us detecting it.

In its quest to save time, the brain abbreviates the second observation, with the potential consequences of approaching traffic, originally far enough away to be detected but not cognitively acknowledged as a hazard, now suddenly “…appearing out of nowhere, officer!”

Reflection does not occur naturally in a useful form for us, so interpreting a back to front reflection of an object requires a cognitively conditioned process, whereby we correctly interpret that ‘left is right’ and ‘right is left’.

In the driven environment, we have a mirror that gives a rearward field of view, generally on a moving object – the side of our vehicle. Moreover, we are also generally looking for, or looking at, moving vehicles.

Designing the mirror glass to be convex, non‑symmetrical, and, possibly with a varying radius across the glass, leads to reflected objects being back to front and distorted, more so toward the edges of the glass.  Recognised research has found that judgement of speed and distance in convex rearview mirrors is problematic, even to the extent that such mirrors are not permitted on the offside of vehicles in the USA.

Mirror observations take time, even more so where a head-turn is required.  After the observation comes cognitive processing time, when although we might now be looking ahead, the brain is still processing and applying the viewed data.

Large goods vehicle drivers take this a stage further – mirrors at the front and nearside of their vehicles that can be reversed images, distorted, and upside down.  All of these processes should be considered in the context of driving within a collision event, especially where the driver’s perception-response time (PRT) is considered with time available.

 

External rear-view mirror illustrating the distance disparity between the image of the white car in the main, planar lens, and the convex top right lens.

 

Perception Response Time – where to start? One of the most researched topics of a collision reconstruction is that time from detection of a hazard to commencement of our response – braking, steering, both, or neither, – to that hazard.

Potentially affected by many factors, such as fatigue and intoxication, but aside from those comes experience, expectancy, and other factors such as distraction, which might include mirror checks.

Expectancy – from detecting an object that is sufficiently contrasting against its background, it still does not follow that the time to action remains constant.  Once an image is processed in the mid-brain and identified, our rationalising functions can question why the identified object is ahead of us. This can also take time. So, it is realistic that the pedestrian standing in the middle of an unlit motorway at night, being ‘out of context’, would result in a slightly longer time for a driver to react than, for example, a pedestrian in a residential street during daytime (in context).

All of these factors have been peer-reviewed researched, and are recognised constructs and influences upon PRT and driver performance.  To not consider in an informed manner these factors in the context of an incident is to fail to serve expert service to the Courts and to the road users involved.

The above is a far from an exhaustive list of factors for the collision investigator to consider, but it offers a small insight into the field of human factors and its influence on a collision reconstruction.

Related posts
Reflecting on the historic pioneering approaches of TRL
July 1, 2022
Pedestrian safety | Airbags
April 7, 2022
TRL Technical Series: Video Analysis | Calculating distance and speed
March 28, 2022
A history of impact biomechanics, Part 2 – from TRL beginnings to the 1990s
March 13, 2022
TRL Technical Series: Load Security
December 14, 2021
A history of impact biomechanics, Part 1 – from the Ancient Greeks to the 1970s
December 14, 2021
Search
Recent Posts
  • Reflecting on the historic pioneering approaches of TRL
  • Human Factors in Collision Investigation
  • Pedestrian safety | Airbags
  • TRL Technical Series: Video Analysis | Calculating distance and speed
  • A history of impact biomechanics, Part 2 – from TRL beginnings to the 1990s
News Archive
  • July 2022
  • June 2022
  • April 2022
  • March 2022
  • December 2021
  • November 2021
  • October 2021
  • June 2021
  • March 2021
  • December 2020
  • October 2020
  • September 2020
  • December 2019
  • September 2019
  • August 2019
  • July 2019
  • May 2019
  • January 2019
  • December 2018
  • November 2018
  • September 2018
  • August 2018
  • July 2018
  • June 2018
  • May 2018
  • April 2018
  • March 2018
  • February 2018
  • January 2018

Proven, Trusted, Effective

To find out more about our Team & the Services we offer, please email investigations@trl.co.uk  or call 01344 770892

Contact Us
Services
  • Collision Investigation & Reconstruction
  • Rapid Response Scene & Vehicle Inspections
  • Initial Appraisal Service
  • CCTV / Dashcam Footage
  • GPS / Cycle Computer Analysis
  • Mechanical Vehicle Inspection
  • EDR / Black Box Downloads
  • Injury Causation, including Seat Belt and Helmet Usage
 
  • 2D Scene Plans
  • 3D Simulation
  • Driver & Vehicle Visibility Assessments
  • Vehicle Infotainment / Telematics System Downloads
  • Tachograph Analysis
  • Vehicle Fire Investigation
  • Vehicle Identification
  • Highways Investigations
  • Collision Investigation Training
Contact Us

01344 770892
investigations@trl.co.uk

Investigations Group
TRL Limited
Crowthorne House
Nine Mile Ride
Wokingham
Berkshire
RG40 3GA

Expert Witness & Investigations Services
TRL Registered Office: Crowthorne House, Nine Mile Ride, Wokingham, Berks, UK, RG40 3GA. Registered in England, No. 3142272, VAT Registration 664 625 321.
© Copyright 2020 TRL. All rights reserved.
  • Terms & Conditions
  • Privacy Notice
  • Accessibility
Boilerplate